hand holding ipad with images
Making mistakes: Children with autism who perform poorly on a test of self-control tend to like routine.

Special interests hinder self-control in children with autism

Children with autism show much less restraint when confronted with one of their interests, compared with their typical peers.

By Hannah Furfaro
12 May 2018 | 3 min read

This article is more than five years old.

Neuroscience—and science in general—is constantly evolving, so older articles may contain information or theories that have been reevaluated since their original publication date.

Children with autism show much less restraint when confronted with one of their interests, compared with their typical peers. And this lack of self-control may interfere with their ability to perform cognitive or everyday tasks.

Researchers presented the unpublished results today at the 2018 International Society for Autism Research annual meeting in Rotterdam, the Netherlands.

The team used a ‘go/no-go’ task to assess the children’s ability to control their impulses. The standard task involves quickly pressing a certain set of images on a screen — and refraining from touching a different set when they pop up.

The researchers used a modified version of this task that involves images of the children’s favorite items or toys, such as airplanes or trains.

Overall, children with autism made more errors on the task than their typical peers when they were shown images of their interests.

“Seeing the interest just really interferes with correctly performing the task at hand,” says Dienke Bos, a postdoctoral researcher in Rebecca Jones’ lab at Weill Cornell Medicine in New York, who presented the work.

Tapping trouble:

The researchers recruited 62 children with autism and 38 typical children, aged 10 years on average. They asked the children to choose the image they liked the least and most from a set of 23 images on a tablet.

In some rounds, they asked the children to tap the screen as quickly as possible when they saw their favorite item appear (often), but avoid tapping the other images (rare). In other rounds, they asked the children to tap images they were not interested in (often), and withhold the tap when their interest appeared (rare).

Typical children performed with about the same accuracy no matter what the screen displayed. But children with autism performed poorly whenever the screen showed their interests.

The children with autism who performed most poorly score high on a measure of ‘insistence on sameness’ on the Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised, a parent questionnaire. There was no relationship between their performance and a subscale that measures sensory-motor behaviors, suggesting that the relationship with sameness is specific to this lack of self-control.

The relationship between insistence on sameness and cognitive control has been “very hard to piece out in the past,” Bos says. “Introducing these interests in the task brings us closer to connecting these two elements — cognitive control as we see it in the lab, and rigid behaviors as we see it in real life.”

The findings suggest that using a child’s special interests as motivation may be counterproductive during therapy: Something as simple as the presence of a child’s favorite toy in the room could be a distraction.

For more reports from the 2018 International Society for Autism Research annual meeting, please click here.

Sign up for the weekly Spectrum newsletter.

Stay current with the latest advancements in autism research.