Broken mirrors

Research into the ability of children with autism to imitate has produced contradictory results: Some studies find that those with the disorder have difficulty imitating people, whereas others find no problems. A new study explores why.

By Emily Singer
17 August 2012 | 2 min read

This article is more than five years old.

Neuroscience—and science in general—is constantly evolving, so older articles may contain information or theories that have been reevaluated since their original publication date.

Do children with autism have difficulty imitating the actions of others? The answer is hotly debated in the field.

Proponents of the so-called broken mirror hypothesis contend that people with autism have faulty mirror neurons, cells that are typically active when mimicking others. Problems with the mirror neuron system could explain many of the social deficits linked to autism, these researchers say.

However, research exploring this idea has had mixed results. Some studies show children with autism have trouble imitating people, but others suggest they behave similarly to controls.

A new study, published 18 July in Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, suggests that problems with imitation may be limited to emotional contexts, and may explain the seemingly contradictory results.

Imitation is thought to be an essential part of human interaction and communication. People naturally mimic the facial expressions of those around them, even if not asked to do so.

In the new study of 15 high-functioning children with autism and 15 controls, researchers found that both groups are equally able to imitate a finger movement performed by someone with a neutral facial expression. However, the controls perform much better when the person doing the finger task has an emotional expression rather than a neutral one. The autism group, in contrast, shows no change.

In a second experiment, the same groups of children had to name different facial expressions and then mimic them. Children with autism are able to name emotions linked to facial expressions almost as well as controls are. (They are worse at naming happy expressions, a difference that may be the result of weaker language skills in these children.) But the children with autism have a much more difficult time imitating fearful facial expressions than controls do.

Taken together, the results suggest that children with autism can imitate movements and recognize facial expressions as well as controls can, but they have problems when these two skills are put together — imitating others in a social context.

Sign up for the weekly Spectrum newsletter.

Stay current with the latest advancements in autism research.