What ‘drifting representations’ reveal about the brain

How neuronal activity patterns associated with a behavior can change, even when the behavior does not — something once seen as an experimental artifact — could offer new insights about neural function.

An abstract illustration of a figure from the shoulders up with multi-colored boxes on its face
Shifty activity: Neurons sometimes change their response patterns over time — even when everything else seems to be stable.
Illustration by Ibrahim Rayintakath

Dozens of green and red magnets littered the three whiteboards in the meeting room. Each one signified a vote — a yea or nay on various claims about a puzzling neural phenomenon called “representational drift.” Put simply, representational drift describes the observation that, over time, a group of neurons can change their response to some stimulus, despite no apparent changes in the animal’s perception or behavior.

As the scattered magnets suggested, there was little consensus among the roughly 100 neuroscientists in the room, who gathered this past April for a four-day meeting at the Janelia Research Campus in Ashburn, Virginia.

“In some people’s eyes, [representational drift] is sort of a controversial idea,” says Timothy O’Leary, professor of information engineering and neuroscience at the University of Cambridge in the United Kingdom, who helped to organize the meeting and recounted the whiteboard poll.

That’s in part because representational drift defies expectations that stable behaviors should derive from stable neuronal activity. So much so that when several independent teams first identified it nearly two decades ago, many dismissed it as experimental error: Perhaps the electrodes recording the neuronal activity had slipped over time and started recording different cells, for example, or the data had been analyzed improperly.

In recent years, however, other researchers have confirmed some of those early findings — thanks to technology advancements that help experimentalists track the responses of single neurons over the course of many months. The new findings spurred O’Leary and his co-organizers to plan the standalone April meeting. From O’Leary’s vantage, representational drift is akin to how a nation’s foreign policy can remain stable across decades, even as the elected individuals who shape that policy enter and leave office.

The vote at the April meeting came before the talks began, and attendees proffered a range of possible mechanistic explanations, which the organizers wrote on the whiteboard — is representational drift an “unavoidable side effect of ongoing learning,” as some suggested, or a “way of encoding information,” as others voted? A minority was skeptical in general, with five attendees voting that drift is either “largely a measurement artifact” or “a result of low statistical power/poor analysis.” (“I don’t want to believe that, but I do,” responded one attendee, as memorialized in a photograph of the whiteboard.)

But by the time the meeting was over, the number who definitively saw drift as an artifact or analysis problem had dropped to zero. The evidence for drift being a fundamental property of the brain is mounting, O’Leary says. And despite the lingering doubts, the meeting signaled a shift in how neuroscientists are thinking about representational drift.

“The bottom line is that the data now forces people to confront this phenomenon,” says meeting co-organizer Andrew Fink, a postdoctoral researcher in Richard Axel’s lab at Columbia University. “It’s hard to argue that it doesn’t exist. And so now we’re tasked with thinking about what it means.” 

A

s researchers probe the brain for signs of representational drift, a pattern has started to emerge: Some brain areas seem to exhibit more drift than others.

Place cells in the CA1 region of the hippocampus, for example, exhibit significant drift, as do neurons in the posterior parietal cortex, two mouse studies suggest. Even after the mice learned to navigate their environment or perform a particular task, the associated neuronal activity in either brain area changed over time. Learning seemed to have stopped, yet the animals’ brain responses continued to change.

The piriform cortex, which is involved in olfaction, is another hub for representational drift, according to a 2021 mouse study. Electrodes implanted in that brain region stably recorded neuronal activity as the animals were exposed to odors over 32 days. Looking at the activity of all recorded cells, the researchers could identify which odor the animal was smelling, but the individual cells that made up that response changed over the course of the study.

“Our interpretation of that data is that it reflects some ongoing learning process,” even after the animals had seemingly become familiar with an odor, says Fink, lead researcher on the study.

Not all brain areas need to participate in such flexible learning, and so, the theory goes, not all exhibit the same amount of drift. Drift in the auditory, visual or somatosensory cortex seems to happen on a smaller scale than it does in the hippocampus or posterior parietal cortex, for example, O’Leary says. Cells in the rat motor cortex remain stable over long periods of time, according to 2022 findings. And neurons that respond to faces show hardly any drift at all, retaining the same responses for up to a year, according to a 2014 study in monkeys.

The baseline amount of drift across various regions seems to provide a measurable signal of plasticity at the level of a neural population, O’Leary says. And studying it “gives you a quantitative window into what’s happening in different parts of the brain.”

How much randomness, or noise, is present in the neural signal may account for these differing amounts of drift, although the mechanisms remain unclear.

According to one hypothesis, “drift is a byproduct of noisy learning” — meaning that neurons continue to respond to the randomness within an animal’s environment, says Farhad Pashakhanloo, a postdoctoral fellow at Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory in New York. In a presentation at the Society for Neuroscience meeting last month, Pashakhanloo and his colleagues proposed a model in which the randomness in the environment leads to a certain amount of randomness in neuronal responses, and that, in turn, nudges the cells to drift together in a way that maintains stable task performance, he says.

Some parts of the brain, such as the retina or the motor system, may have had evolutionary pressure to permit less randomness in their neuronal responses, leading to less drift, Fink says. “In areas where there is less of a constraint on stability, maybe that noise is more tolerated.”

“It is a conceptual challenge for how the brain works.”

Leenoy Meshulam

Cells in the retina, for example, need not — and in fact should not — respond to changes in the environment; the relationship between their input and their firing must remain consistent day to day so that an animal can successfully move through the world. The hippocampus, on the other hand, has neurons that constantly adapt their signals, such as remapping spatial context when a mouse is placed in a new chamber.

If drift does result from “noisy learning,” studying drift can help researchers understand the rules behind how learning happens in the brain, Pashankhanloo says. But these theories still need to be tested, he adds. “There may be something else going on.”

W

ithin this relatively new area of research, plenty of questions remain. “It is a conceptual challenge for how the brain works,” says Leenoy Meshulam, a postdoctoral fellow at the University of Washington in Seattle, and a co-organizer of the April meeting. As someone who builds models of the brain, she is still uncertain about how to incorporate drift into her models. “Do you ignore it? Do you take this as a main hallmark of what the system is actually doing? Both these things seem to be irrelevant options at this point, because we don’t know.”

Another hurdle in the field is language. Researchers still do not agree on how to define a “representation” or “drift” — let alone “representational drift.”

When scientists compare fuzzy definitions across studies, the results blur even more. Researchers use different experimental tasks to study drift in the hippocampus versus the visual cortex, and rodents and primates may have fundamental differences in how their neurons respond to various stimuli over time. (It could make perfect sense, ecologically, for a monkey’s brain to require more stable representations of faces than a rodent’s does, for instance, O’Leary says.)

“The problem with having this sort of word or phrase as an attractor is that you have people who are talking about just fundamentally different systems and different processes who are now talking past each other,” says David Leopold, senior investigator at the National Institutes of Mental Health, who attended the meeting.

And, Leopold says, it may not make sense to expect neurons to have unchanging responses to stable conditions. “The brain’s job is to adapt to things, and it does it in different ways,” even when no formal learning is happening, he says. “If you’re going to name any violation of the null hypothesis as representational drift, I’m not that crazy about the entire framework.”

Despite the disagreements, the April event was a step in the right direction, Leopold and other attendees say. One of the goals of the meeting was to establish some baselines for how to measure and report drift in experiments, Meshulam says. “People were reporting different things across the board,” making it difficult to compare across studies, she says. But now, she says, the field has some common ground.

And as the work continues, more answers may come, O’Leary says. “The only way [forward] is the hard way.” That means putting “pressure on experimentalists to do longer recordings, and in more tightly controlled conditions, as well as exploring what happens under less controlled or naturalistic conditions.”

Or as Fink puts it: “What everyone wants is more data.”

Sign up for our weekly newsletter.

Catch up on what you may have missed from our recent coverage.